Abramov, I., Gordon, J., Feldman, O., & Chavarga, A. (2012). Sex & vision i: Spatio-temporal resolution.
Biology of Sex Differences,
3(1), 20.
https://doi.org/10.1186/2042-6410-3-20
Baker, M. (2016). 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility.
Nature News,
533(7604), 452.
https://doi.org/10.1038/533452a
Errington, T. M., Denis, A., Perfito, N., Iorns, E., & Nosek, B. A. (2021). Challenges for assessing replicability in preclinical cancer biology.
eLife,
10, e67995.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67995
Gabelica, M., Bojčić, R., & Puljak, L. (2022). Many researchers were not compliant with their published data sharing statement: A mixed-methods study.
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology,
150, 33–41.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2022.05.019
Gilmore, R. O., & Adolph, K. E. (2017). Video can make behavioural research more reproducible.
Nature Human Behavior,
1.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0128
Gilmore, R. O., & Kohler, P. J. (n.d.). Symmetry-sorting: Behavioral studies associated with symmetry project. Github. Retrieved from
https://github.com/gilmore-lab/symmetry-sorting
Goodman, S. N., Fanelli, D., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2016). What does research reproducibility mean?
Science Translational Medicine,
8(341), 341ps12–341ps12.
https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf5027
Kohler, P. J., Vedak, S., & Gilmore, R. O. (2022). Perceptual similarities among wallpaper group exemplars.
Symmetry,
14(5), 857.
https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14050857
Korbmacher, M., Azevedo, F., Pennington, C. R., Hartmann, H., Pownall, M., Schmidt, K., … Evans, T. (2023). The replication crisis has led to positive structural, procedural, and community changes.
Communications Psychology,
1(1), 1–13.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-023-00003-2
Merton, R. W. (1973). The normative structure of science. In R. K. Merton & N. W. Storer (Eds.), The sociology of science: Theoretical and empirical investigations (pp. 267–278). The University of Chicago Press.
Mischel, W. (2011). Becoming a cumulative science.
APS Observer,
22(1). Retrieved from
https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/becoming-a-cumulative-science
Mitroff, I. I. (1974). Norms and counter-norms in a select group of the apollo moon scientists: A case study of the ambivalence of scientists.
American Sociological Review,
39(4), 579–595.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2094423
Miyakawa, T. (2020). No raw data, no science: Another possible source of the reproducibility crisis.
Molecular Brain,
13(1), 24.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-020-0552-2
Munafò, M. R., Nosek, B. A., Bishop, D. V. M., Button, K. S., Chambers, C. D., Sert, N. P. du, … Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). A manifesto for reproducible science.
Nature Human Behaviour,
1, 0021.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0021
Murray, S. O., Schallmo, M.-P., Kolodny, T., Millin, R., Kale, A., Thomas, P., … Tadin, D. (2018). Sex differences in visual motion processing.
Current Biology: CB.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.014
Oransky, I., & Marcus, A. (2023). Science corrects itself, right? A scandal at stanford says it doesn’t.
Scientific American. Retrieved from
https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/science-corrects-itself-right-a-scandal-at-stanford-says-it-doesnt/
Posit team. (2023).
RStudio: Integrated development environment for r. Boston, MA: Posit Software, PBC. Retrieved from
http://www.posit.co/
Qian, Y., Berenbaum, S. A., & Gilmore, R. O. (2022). Vision contributes to sex differences in spatial cognition and activity interests.
Scientific Reports,
12(1), 17623.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-22269-y
Qian, Y., Seisler, A. R., & Gilmore, R. O. (2021). Children’s perceptual sensitivity to optic flow-like visual motion differs from adults.
Developmental Psychology,
57(11), 1810–1821.
https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001227
Saul, S. (2023). Stanford president will resign after report found flaws in his research.
The New York Times. Retrieved from
https://www.nytimes.com/2023/07/19/us/stanford-president-resigns-tessier-lavigne.html
Shaqiri, A., Roinishvili, M., Grzeczkowski, L., Chkonia, E., Pilz, K., Mohr, C., … Herzog, M. H. (2018). Sex-related differences in vision are heterogeneous.
Scientific Reports,
8(1), 7521.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25298-8
Soska, K. C., Xu, M., Gonzalez, S. L., Herzberg, O., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Gilmore, R. O., & Adolph, K. E. (2021). (Hyper)active data curation: A video case study from behavioral science.
Journal of Escience Librarianship,
10(3).
https://doi.org/10.7191/jeslib.2021.1208
Szucs, D., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature.
PLoS Biology,
15(3), e2000797.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000797
Tedersoo, L., Küngas, R., Oras, E., Köster, K., Eenmaa, H., Leijen, Ä., … Sepp, T. (2021). Data sharing practices and data availability upon request differ across scientific disciplines.
Scientific Data,
8(1), 192.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-021-00981-0
Vanpaemel, W., Vermorgen, M., Deriemaecker, L., & Storms, G. (2015). Are we wasting a good crisis? The availability of psychological research data after the storm.
Collabra,
1(1).
https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.13
Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D., Kats, J., & Molenaar, D. (2006). The poor availability of psychological research data for reanalysis.
The American Psychologist,
61(7), 726–728.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726