2017-11-02 08:15:42

Support


Talk overview

  • Project status report
    • The development of optic flow processing
    • Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project
    • The Proximal Emotional Environment Project (PEEP)
  • Toward an open, transparent, robust, and integrative developmental science

The development of optic flow processing

Questions about optic flow

  • What is optic flow?
  • Why is optic flow important?
  • How does optic flow sensitivity develop?
  • How do brain systems for processing optic flow develop?
  • What shapes these patterns of development?

Approach

  • EEG measures of brain responses to optic flow
  • Psychophysical (behavioral) measures of optic flow perception
  • Empirical measures of experienced optic flow across development from head-mounted video cameras and computer vision analyses

What is Optic Flow?

  • Structured pattern of visual motion generated by observer movement

Types of Optic Flow

Why is optic flow important?

  • Geometry of environment
    • Surface layout, orientation
    • Object motion
  • Visual proprioception
    • Rotation, translation

Flow types specify self-motion

How Does Optic Flow Sensitivity Develop?

How Does Optic Flow Sensitivity Develop?

How Does Optic Flow Sensitivity Develop?

What about older children?

  • Present: Time-varying optic flow patterns
  • Measure: Steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs)
    • Event-related electro-encephalograms (EEGs)
    • Phase-locked responses at low-order harmonics
  • \(n=29\) 4-8 year-olds
  • (R. Gilmore, Thomas, & Fesi, 2016)

2 deg/s translation

4 deg/s rotation

8 deg/s radial

1F1 Channel-Wise Results

1F1 Channels p < .0005

Complex Domain Plot of 1F1 Channels

3F1 Channel-Wise Results

3F1 Channels p < .0005

Complex Domain Plot of 3F1 Channels

Results Summary

  • Anatomical & frequency separation of responses
    • vs. pattern (lateral, 1F1)
    • speed (medial, 3F1)
  • Radial & rotation \(\neq\) translation
  • Speed tuning (slow < med & fast)
  • Similar to, but different from adults

Children's 1F1

Adults' 1F1

Children's 3F1

Adults' 3F1

What about behavior?

  • Time-varying optic flow
    • Radial, linear
    • {2 deg/s, 8 deg/s}
    • {5, 10, 15, 20%} coherence (adults)
    • {15, 30, 45, 60%} and {20, 40, 60, 80%} (children)

Methods

Children's responses p(correct)

Adults' responses p(correct)

Speed effects in children

Speed effects in adults

Pattern effects in children

Pattern effects in adults

Behavioral Summary

  • Children's behavior: more accurate to detect fast speeds, radial patterns
  • Adults more accurate to detect slow speeds, radial patterns
  • Response speeds in children and adults (not shown) show similar patterns
  • But, why?

Potential factors shaping development of flow sensitivity

  • External
    • Environment
  • Internal
    • Posture
    • Locomotion, head, eye movements

Head mounted eye tracker data from "coupled" infant/mom dyads

Adolph, K. (2015). Active vision in passive locomotion: real-world free viewing in infants and adults. Databrary. Retrieved February 18, 2017 from http://doi.org/10.17910/B7.123

Synchronized infant and mother cams

Findings

Findings

  • Infant (passengers) experience faster motion speeds than mothers
  • Controlling for speed of locomotion, environment

Replication

  • Do the findings hold in larger samples
  • When geometry of environment varies
  • Outside the lab
  • Across age

Experienced flow across cultures

Participant summary

Illustrative Speed Histograms - 6 weeks

Illustrative Speed Histograms – 34 weeks

Illustrative Speed Histograms – 58 weeks

Pattern Correlation Results

Conclusions: Measuring experienced flow

  • Fast speeds, broad speed distributions
  • Linear flow >> radial or rotational flow

Simulating developmental change

\(\begin{pmatrix}\dot{x} \\ \dot{y}\end{pmatrix}=\frac{1}{z} \begin{pmatrix}-f & 0 & x\\ 0 & -f & y \end{pmatrix} \begin{pmatrix}{v_x{}}\\ {v_y{}} \\{v_z{}}\end{pmatrix}+ \frac{1}{f} \begin{pmatrix} xy & -(f^2+x^2) & fy\\ f^2+y^2 & -xy & -fy \end{pmatrix} \begin{pmatrix} \omega_{x}\\ \omega_{y}\\ \omega_{z} \end{pmatrix}\)

Geometry of environment/observer: \((x, y, z)\)
Translational speed: \((v_x, v_y, v_z)\)
Rotational speed: \((\omega_{x}, \omega_{y}, \omega{z})\)
Retinal flow: \((\dot{x}, \dot{y})\)

Parameters For Simulation

Parameter Crawling Infant Walking Infant
Eye height 0.30 m 0.60 m
Locomotor speed 0.33 m/s 0.61 m/s
Head tilt 20 deg 9 deg

Geometric Feature Distance
Side wall +/- 2 m
Side wall height 2.5 m
Distance of ground plane 32 m
Field of view width 60 deg
Field of view height 45 deg

Simulating Flow Fields

Simulated Flow Speeds (m/s)

Type of Locomotion Ground Plane Room Side Wall Two Walls
Crawling 14.41 14.42 14.43 14.62
Walking 9.38 8.56 7.39 9.18

Summing up

  • Infants commonly experience fast, linear optic flows
    • Head/eye instability
    • Proximity to ground
  • Brain and behavioral responses to optic flow develop throughout childhood
    • Still immature in 5-8 year-olds
  • Statistics of natural experience informative

Some next steps

  • Individual differences in flow sensitivity
  • Brain responses to flow 'in the wild'
  • Comparing brain responses to behavioral sensitivity
  • Simulating the effects of development on visual experience
  • Automating pipelines for automated analysis of 1st person videos (using PSU's Galaxy environment)

An hour in the life…

Play & Learning Across a Year project

  • Play is the central context and activity of early development
  • But what do parents and infants actually do when they 'play'?

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • \(n=900\) infant/mother dyads; 300 @ 12-, 18-, 24-months
  • 30 dyads from 30 sites across the US
  • 1 hr natural activity
    • 3 min solitary toy play
    • 2 min dyadic toy play
    • video tour of home

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • Demographics + parent-report questionnaires about health, family, temperament
  • Ambient sound levels

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • Census block group geocoding

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • "Big data" developmental science

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • Data shared on Databrary

Play & Learning Across a Year (PLAY) project

  • Video as data and documentation

What questions would you ask about these data?

The Proximal Emotional Environment Project (PEEP)

Guess the emotion

Sample 1 Sample 2

Questions

  • How do children's brains respond to overheard speech with different affective prosodies?
  • How do children's brains respond to different speakers?
  • How do children perceive the affective qualities of overheard speech?
  • What are the acoustic properties that distinguish affectively-laden speech?
  • What is the natural "affective" environment

How 'angry' are the scenarios

How 'happy' are they?

'Intensity' ratings across time

What's in common?

Themes

  • Natural environments are rich
  • Perception, action, cognition & emotion inextricably linked
  • Need better/denser measures
    • of environment
    • of behavior
    • of physiology

Turtles networks all the way down…

Power of

  • convergent evidence
  • data visualization
  • data, materials sharing
  • to accelerate discovery…

Limitations of

  • scientific culture
  • technology
  • sampling diversity, sample sizes/power, publication bias, etc.
  • big team science vs. small team science

Toward an open, transparent, robust, and integrative developmental science

What would a data observatory for human development look like?

Combine data from diverse domains

Link measures across people

Enable web-based data visualization, analysis

Support search, filtering by personal characteristics

Encourage self/active curation of data, materials

Provide consistent, clear sharing permissions structure

Progress

Example Multi-measure Indiv link/search Visualize Self-curate Permissions
Databrary tabular
Human Proj ? ?
ICPSR ? ?
Neurosynth fMRI BOLD group data public NA
OpenNeuro ? public
Open Humans ? ?
OSF public
WordBank M-CDI group metadata ? public

Barriers

Humans are diverse

  • But much (lab-based) data collected are from Western, Educated Industrialized, Rich, Democratic (WEIRD) populations Henrich et al., 2010

Data sensitive, hard(er) to share

  • Protect participant's identities
  • Protect from harm/embarrassment
  • Anonymize (effective?) or get permission (many don't ask, plan to share)

"…psychologists tend to treat other peoples’ theories like toothbrushes; no self-respecting individual wants to use anyone else’s."

Walter Mischel, 2009

"Reviewers and editors want novel, interesting results. Why would I waste my time doing careful direct replications?"

Any number of researchers I've talked with

Let's build a Databservatory for human behavior

It should…

  • Store & share data & materials
  • Link data across studies, measures
  • Link across group characteristics, individuals
  • Enable searching & filtering by individual characteristics, tasks, settings

  • Support web-based data analysis, visualization; open API
  • Provide consistent framework for ethical data sharing
  • Enable data aggregation, cloning, provenance tracking
  • Support self/active curation
  • Link to publications &…

Let us embrace the whole elephant

Stack

This talk was produced on 2017-11-02 in RStudio using R Markdown and the ioslides framework. The code and materials used to generate the slides may be found at https://github.com/gilmore-lab/2017-10-18-the-whole-elephant/. Information about the R Session that produced the code is as follows:

sessionInfo()
## R version 3.4.1 (2017-06-30)
## Platform: x86_64-apple-darwin15.6.0 (64-bit)
## Running under: macOS Sierra 10.12.6
## 
## Matrix products: default
## BLAS: /Library/Frameworks/R.framework/Versions/3.4/Resources/lib/libRblas.0.dylib
## LAPACK: /Library/Frameworks/R.framework/Versions/3.4/Resources/lib/libRlapack.dylib
## 
## locale:
## [1] en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8/C/en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8
## 
## attached base packages:
## [1] stats     graphics  grDevices utils     datasets  methods   base     
## 
## loaded via a namespace (and not attached):
##  [1] compiler_3.4.1  backports_1.1.0 magrittr_1.5    rprojroot_1.2  
##  [5] tools_3.4.1     htmltools_0.3.6 yaml_2.1.14     Rcpp_0.12.12   
##  [9] stringi_1.1.5   rmarkdown_1.6   knitr_1.17      stringr_1.2.0  
## [13] digest_0.6.12   evaluate_0.10.1

References

Adamiak, W., Thomas, A., Patel, S., & Gilmore, R. (2015). Adult Observer’s Sensitivity to Optic Flow Varies by Pattern and Speed. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 1008. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.12.1008

Gilmore, R. O., & Adolph, K. E. (2017). Video can make behavioural science more reproducible. Nature Human Behavior, 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0128

Gilmore, R. O., Raudies, F., & Jayaraman, S. (2015). What accounts for developmental shifts in optic flow sensitivity? In 2015 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob) (pp. 19–25). https://doi.org/10.1109/DEVLRN.2015.7345450

Gilmore, R., Hou, C., Pettet, M., & Norcia, A. (2007). Development of cortical responses to optic flow. Visual Neuroscience, 24(06), 845–856. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523807070769

Gilmore, R., Thomas, A., & Fesi, J. (2016). Children’s Brain Responses to Optic Flow Vary by Pattern Type and Motion Speed. PLOS ONE, 11(6), e0157911. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157911

Hou, C., Gilmore, R., Pettet, M., & Norcia, A. (2009). Spatio-temporal tuning of coherent motion evoked responses in 4–6 month old infants and adults. Vision Research, 49(20), 2509–2517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.007

Jouen, F., Lepecq, J.-C., Gapenne, O., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2000). Optic flow sensitivity in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(3–4), 271–284. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00044-3

Kiorpes, L., & Movshon, J. A. (2004). Development of sensitivity to visual motion in macaque monkeys. Visual Neuroscience, 21(6), 851–859. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523804216054

LeWinn, K. Z., Sheridan, M. A., Keyes, K. M., Hamilton, A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat. Commun., 8(1), 874. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-00908-7

Raudies, F., & Gilmore, R. (2014). Visual Motion Priors Differ for Infants and Mothers. Neural Computation, 26(11), 2652–2668. https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00645

Yu, C. P., Page, W. K., Gaborski, R., & Duffy, C. J. (2010). Receptive Field Dynamics Underlying MST Neuronal Optic Flow Selectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(5), 2794–2807. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01085.2009